ŋaren crîþa 9 vlefto: Ŋarâþ Crîþ v9

Morphological paradigms

Each lexical entry belongs to a lexical category (nouns, verbs, &c.). Each lexical category is associated with one or more paradigms, each of which has

For instance, the lexical category of nouns contains one paradigm named default with a schema of (case:Case,number:Nnumber).

An instance of a schema is a named tuple, each of whose elements has a name identical to that of the corresponding element in the schema and a value in the set of values of the respective grammatical category. For example, (case:nominative,number:dual) is an instance of the schema (case:Case,number:Nnumber).

Given a lexical category C consisting of k paradigms P0=(n0,σ0,f0),,Pk1, a lexical entry L with a lexical category of C consists of k paradigm applications A0=(n0,s0),,Ak1, where si=fi(,L) is a function mapping instances of σi to a set of strings. For the noun ⟨ŋarâþ⟩, the default paradigm application maps (case:nominative,number:dual) to {ŋarac}.

How exactly the mappings of paradigms are defined in Ŋarâþ Crîþ is described in Morphophonology.

Grammatical categories

A grammatical category consists of a set of two or more values. Grammatical categories are usually used as inputs to paradigm mappings but occasionally also appear as intrinsic properties of lexical entries.

Number

Ŋarâþ Crîþ has separate ideas of nominal and verbal numbers, which are often abbreviated as nnumbers and as vnumbers. Nominal numbers are used in the declension of nouns, while verbal numbers are used for agreement affixes in verbs and relationals. There are four verbal numbers: singular, dual, plural, and generic.

There are five nominal numbers: direct, dual, plural, singulative, and generic. No noun can decline for all five nnumbers; instead, each noun is limited to a subset of these according to its clareþ, which governs how nominal numbers map to verbal numbers:

Generic number is used to mean “X in general” or “X as a concept”. It is used on noun phrases that do not refer to a specific referent or referents:

(1)
mjoþelca glefteþ @asares tferamotras menu.
mjoþ-elca
gender-inst.sg
glev-þeþ
discrimination-dat.gc
@asar-es
Asoren-loc.sg
tfera-motr-as
often-ddt-loc.sg
men-u.
see-3sg
Discrimination based on gender is being seen increasingly often in Asoren.
(metions discrimination in general)
(2)
«ranaren gleverþ plence» reþ teno gcarþeþ.
«ran-ar-en
3pl.hum-toward-adn
glev-erþ
discrimination-nom.di
plenc-e»
illegal-sg
reþ
quot.acc.ind
ten-o
court-nom.sg
g\carþ-e-þ.
pfv\declare-3sg-pst
The court ruled that the discimination against them violated the law.
(metions a specific case of discrimination)
(3)
telu tovrełen mênču.
tel-u
fish-nom.gc
tovr-ełen
flower-acc.gc
mênč-u.
eat-3gc
Fish eat flowers.
(general truth, so “fish” would be in the generic and “flowers” would be in the generic)
(4)
telos tovrełen mênči.
tel-os
fish-nom.co
tovr-ełen
flower-acc.gc
mênč-i.
eat-3pl
The fish eat flowers.
(referring to a particular group of fish, but no particular group of flowers, perhaps indicating a habitual action)
(5)
telos tovran mênči.
tel-os
fish-nom.co
tovr-an
flower-acc.co
mênč-i.
eat-3pl
The fish eat (some/the) flowers.
(referring to particular groups of fish and of flowers)
(6)
telu vônos respos tovran vilhenrotomin mênču.
tel-u
fish-nom.gc
vôn-os
norm-loc.di
resp-os
lifetime-loc.sg
tovr-an
flower-acc.sg
vil-henroto-min
one-16².approx-ctr.small_plant
mênč-u.
eat-3gc
A fish will usually eat about 256 flowers during its lifespan.
(“Fish” is generic, as this sentence is not referring to a particular fish but rather an idealized individual reflecting the average. “Flowers” is plural and “lifespan” is singular because they are definite relative to the subject, even though the subject is generic.)
(7)
elêþ šileħe.
el-êþ
sun-nom.sg
šile-ħe.
shine-3sg
The sun shines.
Or: The sun is shining.
(The former interpretation states a general fact, but there is only one sun, so ⟨elêþ⟩ still takes the singular number.)

The generic number is also used for noun phrases that do not necessarily have a referent:

(8)
nemir nirþeftês es veła.
nem-ir
apple-nom.sg
nirþev-tês
basket-dat.sg
es
inside
veła.
exist-3sg
(A/The) apple is in the basket.
(9)
nemir nirþeftês es ceła.
nem-ir
apple-nom.sg
nirþev-tês
basket-dat.sg
es
inside
ceła.
not_exist-3sg
The apple is not in the basket.
(10)
nefþes nirþeftês es cir.
nem-þes
apple-nom.gc
nirþev-tês
basket-dat.sg
es
inside
cir.
not_exist-3gc
There is no apple in the basket.
(11)
ša nefþes nirþeftês es ver?
ša
int
nem-þes
apple-nom.gc
nirþev-tês
basket-dat.sg
es
inside
ver?
exist-3gc
Is there an apple in the basket?

When a noun in the genitive case is used for description, it usually takes the direct number, not the generic.

A third-person pronoun in the generic number refers to a class of objects or people in general and can be translated as the English impersonal pronoun one. First-person and second-person generic pronouns act similarly, except that they include first-person or second-person referents. The use of generic-number pronouns is most notable in imperatives:

(12)
le celcols es coclat garu.
le
imp
celc-ols
building-dat.sg
es
inside
cocl-at
run-inf
gar-u.
refrain_from-3gc
No running in the building. (least direct)
(13)
le celcols es coclat garaf.
le
imp
celc-ols
building-dat.sg
es
inside
cocl-at
run-inf
gar-af.
refrain_from-2gc
Don’t run in the building.
(14)
le celcols es coclat garas.
le
imp
celc-ols
building-dat.sg
es
inside
cocl-at
run-inf
gar-as.
refrain_from-2sg
Don’t run in the building. (aimed specifically at one person; most direct)

Case

Ŋarâþ Crîþ has eight cases (Table 1). The nominative, accusative, dative, and genitive cases are considered core cases. In general, the first three of these are used for arguments to verbs, the genitive case for adnominal adjuncts, and the other four cases for adnominal or adverbial adjuncts.

NameUse
NominativeThe subject of the clause. The citation form of a noun is the nominative singular.
AccusativeThe “direct object” of the clause.
DativeThe “indirect object” of the clause. Also used as a vocative.
GenitiveShows such things as possession, composition, description, or apposition.
LocativeIndicates the location or time of an object or an action: at X, on X, in X. On a nominalized verb, this case can be translated as when, where, or as long as.
InstrumentalIndicates the comitative or the instrumental: with X.
AbessiveThe negation of the instrumental: without X. In the dual number, with only one X.
Semblativelike X in behavior. On a nominalized verb, such that, as though, or to the point that (although ⟨dôm⟩ is used more often for the last sense). Not used for semblance in appearance.
Table 1: The cases of Ŋarâþ Crîþ.

Gender

Ŋarâþ Crîþ has three genders or noun classes: celestial, terrestrial, and human. Gender is an intrinsic part of each noun; that is, the gender of each noun is fixed. The unmarked gender is celestial, in that noun phrases are assumed to be celestial unless otherwise specified, and the celestial gender is used for an object whose gender is otherwise unknown (such as in participles of headless relative clauses).

Many words agree with noun phrases in gender when they are inflected, including:

Tense and aspect

Ŋarâþ Crîþ has two tenses: present (more precisely, nonpast) and past. In addition to the present, the present tense covers the future as well as the immediate past.

The present tense is also used as a narrative present: in stories, the past tense is used only for events that had happened before the current point.

Ŋarâþ Crîþ has two aspects: imperfective and perfective. The imperfective aspect is used for ongoing (such as progressive or habitual) actions.

Mood

Ŋarâþ Crîþ inflects for mood only on nominalized forms of a verb in certain cases. The mood distinction encodes a difference in modality in the dative and semblative cases only. In the locative, instrumental, and abessive, it encodes a distinction between adverbial and adnominal forms.

Attachment

Attachment refers to whether an adjunct is attached to a noun phrase or to a verb phrase. This is marked only on relationals, as well as nominalized forms of a verb in cases that use mood distinctions to signal attachment.